Carlos Gershenson (2002)
Philosophical Ideas on the Simulation of Social Behaviour
Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation
vol. 5, no. 3
To cite articles published in the Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, please reference the above information and include paragraph numbers if necessary
<https://www.jasss.org/5/3/8.html>
Received: 21-May-2002 Accepted: 25-Jun-2002 Published: 30-Jun-2002
Figure 1. The inductive method tests a theory of a system by matching predicted facts against observed facts (Steels, 1995). |
Figure 2. The synthetic method builds up theories of a system attempting to construct an artificial system that exhibits the same capabilities of the natural system (Steels, 1995). |
2 Following the concept of purposeful behaviour of Rosenblueth and Wiener (1968).
3 For Castelfranchi (1998) a "social purpose" would be a "social goal".
4 For a general introduction to complex systems, see Bar-Yam (1997)
5 For the ontological difference between the relative being and absolute being, please refer to Gershenson (2001b; 2002).
6 A good example of this can be observed with random boolean networks (Kauffman, 1993; Gershenson, submitted).
7 This can be seen as a version of the "silly theorem problem" (Gershenson, 2001b): for any silly theorem, you can define infinite sets of axioms so that the silly theorem is consistent with the axioms.
8 Less incompletely speaking, we would say that the experience is based on the axioms (beliefs) and the reasonings (logical arguments) built upon the axioms, the reasonings are based upon the beliefs and experience, and beliefs are based upon reasonings and experience (Gershenson, 2002).
9 Again, we would have the "silly theorem problem" (Gershenson, 2001b).
10 This idea is clearly presented by Michael Arbib (1989), speaking about brain models: "a model that simply duplicates the brain is no more illuminating than the brain itself" (p. 8). For understanding complex systems, simplifications are necessary. But we need to justify our simplifications anyway.
11 We would notice that also as the complexity of our cognition is increased, it increases the complexity of the society, stimulating the individuals to increase their complexity as well. It can be seen as a self-reinforcing process or as a positive feedback loop.
12 Not that it cannot be done, but then the complexity of the individual would need to be similar to the complexity of a culture... which for humans, seems not to be the case.
BAR-YAM, Y. (1997). Dynamics of Complex Systems. Addison-Wesley.
BREAZEAL, C. (1999). Imitation as Social Exchange between Humans and Robots. Proceedings of the 1999 Symposium on Imitation in Animals and Artifacts (AISB99), pp. 96-104. Edinburgh, Scotland.
CANGELOSI, A. and D. PARISI (2001) (Eds.). Simulating the Evolution of Language. Springer.
CASTELFRANCHI, C. (1998). Modelling social action for AI agents. Artificial Intelligence 103, pp. 157-182.
CHIALVO, D. R., MILLONAS, M. M. (1995) How swarms build cognitive maps. Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 95-03-033.
COHEN, M. D., R. L. RIOLO, and R. AXELROD (1999) The Emergence of Social Organization in the Prisoner's Dilemma: How Context-Preservation and Other Factors Promote Cooperation. Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 99-01-002.
DAUTENHAHN, K. and C. L. NEHANIV (Eds.) (2002), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. MIT Press.
DI PAOLO, E. A. (2000). Behavioral coordination, structural congruence and entrainment in a simulation of acoustically coupled agents. Adaptive Behavior 8 (1).
DI PAOLO, E. A., J. NOBLE, and S. BULLOCK (2000). Simulation Models as Opaque Thought Experiments. Artificial Life VII, pp. 1-6.
DUNBAR, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6, pp. 178-190.
EPSTEIN, J. M. and R. L. AXTELL (1996). Growing Artificial Societies: Social Science from the Bottom Up. The Brookings Institution Press, Washington, D. C. & The MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.
GERSHENSON, C. (2001a). Artificial Societies of Intelligent Agents. Unpublished BEng Thesis. FundaciÛn Arturo Rosenblueth, MÈxico.
GERSHENSON, C. (2001b). Comments to Neutrosophy. In Smarandache, F. (Ed.) Proceedings of the First International Conference on Neutrosophy, Neutrosophic Logic, Set, Probability and Statistics, University of New Mexico. Gallup, NM.
GERSHENSON, C. (2002). Complex Philosophy. Proceedings of the 1st Biennial Seminar on Philosophical, Methodological & Epistemological Implications of Complexity Theory. La Habana, Cuba.
GERSHENSON, C. (submitted). Classification of Random Boolean Networks.
GOLDSPINK, C. (2000). Modelling Social Systems as Complex: Towards a Social Simulation Meta-model. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 3 (2).
GOLDSPINK, C. (2002). Methodological Implications of Complex Systems Approaches to Sociality: Simulation as a foundation for knowledge. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 5 (1).
HEMELRIJK, C. K. (1997). Cooperation Without Genes, Games Or Cognition. In Husbands, P. and I. Harvey (Eds.) Fourth European Conference on Artificial Life, pp. 511-520. MIT Press.
HEMELRIJK, C. K. (1999). An individual-oriented model of the emergence of despotic and egalitarian societies. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 266, pp. 361-369.
KAUFFMAN, S. A. (1993) The Origins of Order. Oxford University Press.
MAES, P. (1991) (Ed.). Designing Autonomous Agents: Theory and Practice From Biology to Engineering and Back, MIT Press.
MATARIC, M. J. (1995). Designing and Understanding Adaptive Group Behavior, Adaptive Behavior 4(1).
MATURANA, H. R. and F. J. VARELA (1987). The Tree of Knowledge: The Biological Roots of Human Understanding. Shambhala.
POPPER, K. (1934). The Logic of Scientific Discovery.
ROSENBLUETH, A. and N. WIENER (1968). Purposeful and non-purposeful behaviour, in Buckley, W. (ed.), Modern Systems Research for the Behavioural Scientist, Aldine, Chicago.
SEELEY, T. D. and W. F. TOWNE (1991). Collective decision making in honey bees: how colonies choose among nectar sources. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 28, pp. 277-290.
STEELS, L. (1995). Building Agents out of Autonomous Behavior Systems. In Steels, L. and R. Brooks (Eds.). The Artificial Life Route to Artificial Intelligence. Building Embodied Situated Agents. Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc. Hillsdale, NJ.
STEELS, L. (1996). The Spontaneous Self-organization of an Adaptive Language. In Muggleton, S. (ed.) Machine Intelligence 15. Oxford University Press.
STEELS, L. and F. KAPLAN (2002). AIBO's first words. The social learning of language and meaning. Evolution of Communication, 4 (1).
THOMPSON, E. (2001). Empathy and Consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8 (5-7), pp. 1-32.
VELICER, G. J., L. KROOS, and R. E. LENSKI (2000). Developmental cheating in the social bacterium Myxococcus xanthus. Nature 404, pp. 598-601.
VERSCHURE, P. F. M. J. (1998). Synthetic Epistemology: The acquisition, retention, and expression of knowledge in natural and synthetic systems. Proceedings of the 1998 World Conference on Computational Intelligence, WCC '98, Anchorage, Alaska. IEEE. pp. 147-152.
WEBB, B. (2001). Can Robots Make Good Models of Biological Behaviour? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (6).
WILSON, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Harvard University Press.
Return to Contents of this issue
© Copyright Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, [2002]