Hans-Joachim Mosler (2006)
Better Be Convincing or Better Be Stylish? a Theory Based Multi-Agent Simulation to Explain Minority Influence in Groups Via Arguments or Via Peripheral Cues
Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation
vol. 9, no. 3
<https://www.jasss.org/9/3/4.html>
For information about citing this article, click here
Received: 05-Jan-2006 Accepted: 05-Apr-2006 Published: 30-Jun-2006
Figure 1. Block diagram of the Elaboration Likelihood Model (Petty and Cacioppo 1986). The variables are labelled on the arrows. The transition functions are found in the numbered yellow blocks and explained in the text |
Table 1: Scaling of independent and dependent variables in alphabetical order indicating the terminology used in this paper | |||||||||||
Variable / Scale | 0 | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
(Source) Argument Quality | Low | Indifferent | High | ||||||||
(Source) Attitude | Disagreement | Neutral | Agreement | ||||||||
Bias Position | Negative bias | Neutral | Positive bias | ||||||||
Distraction | None | Maximum | |||||||||
Elaboration Ability | None | Maximum | |||||||||
Elaboration Likelihood | Low | High | |||||||||
Elaboration Motivation | None | Maximum | |||||||||
Knowledge | None | Maximum | |||||||||
Peripheral Cue | Negative | Indifferent | Positive | ||||||||
Personal Relevance | None | Indifferent | Maximum | ||||||||
Value Orientation | Disagreement | Neutral | Agreement | ||||||||
Elaboration motivation = Personal relevance | (1) |
Elaboration ability = Knowledge × {1 - (distraction / 10)} | (2) |
IEL = (Elaboration Motivation * Elaboration Ability) 0.5 | (3a) |
EL = IEL + {[(peripheral cue - 5) / 2] * [1 - |IEL - 5| / 5]} | (3b) |
Bias position = (Attitude + Value orientation) / 2 | (4) |
Central factor = [(SAQ - 5) / 2.5] + {[(5 - SAQ) / 5 + d] * (|BP - 5| / 5) 2} where d = +2, if (BP ≥ 5 and source attitude ≥ 5) or if (BP ≤ 5 and source attitude ≤ 5) where d = -2, if (BP > 5 and source attitude < 5) or if (BP < 5 and source attitude > 5) |
(5) |
Peripheral factor = (peripheral cue) / 1.65 | (6) |
IA = IAt-1 + [(SA — IAt-1) / 3] * {[(EL / 10) * CF] + [(1 — EL / 10) * PF]} | (7) |
PP = (SAQ — AQt-1) / 10 | (8a) |
CP = (SAQ — AQ t-1) / 10 | (8b) |
AQ = AQt-1 + [(EL / 10) * CP] + [(1 — EL / 10) * PP] | (8c) |
Knowledge = Argument quality | (9) |
Figure 2. Sequence of social influence in a group of five agents (A, B, C, D, and E). Red arrows represent social influence. Black arrows represent transitions of agent states (e.g. attitudes) |
Table 2. Attitude change (attitude at the end — attitude at the beginning [= 5]) of the majority in simulation experiment 1 with a minority/majority ratio of one to four. The green cells represent conditions where bipolarisation has occurred, the yellow cells represent conditions where a consensus has occurred |
Table 3. Attitude change (attitude at the end — attitude at the beginning [= 5]) of the majority in simulation experiment 1 with a minority/majority ratio of two to three. The green cells represent conditions where bipolarisation occurred, the yellow cells represent conditions where consensus occurred |
Table 4. Attitude change of the majority in simulation experiment 2. The coloured or framed cells are referred to in the text |
Figure 3. Development of attitudes in the two conditions with red-framed cells in Table 4. The two conditions are identical with the exception of the personal relevance of the minority (high vs. low) |
Table 5. Average change of majority attitude in simulation experiment 3 |
Figure 4. Development of minority and majority attitudes with a slightly negative bias position of the majority and a positive bias position of the majority |
CHAIKEN S (1980) Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752 — 766.
CLARK R and Maass A (1988) The role of social categorisation and perceived source credibility in minority influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 381 — 394.
CRANO W and Hannula-Bral K (1994) Context — Categorisation Model of social influence: Minority and majority influence in the formation of a novel response form. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30 (3), 247 — 276.
DEUTSCH M and Gerard H B (1955) A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629 — 636.
FESTINGER L (1954) A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117 — 140.
JOHNSON B T and Eagly A H (1989) Effects of involvement on persuasion: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 290 — 314.
LATANÉ B (1981) The psychology of social impact. American Psychologist, 86 (4), 343 — 356.
MAASS A and Clark III R (1984) Hidden impact of minorities: Fifteen years of minority influence research. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 428 — 450.
MOSCOVICI S (1980) Towards a theory of conversion behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press.
MOSLER H-J, Ammann F and Gutscher H (1998) Simulation des Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM). Ein Beispiel zur Entwicklung und Analyse von Umweltinterventionen [Using simulation of the Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) to develop and analyse environmental interventions]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 29, 20 — 37.
MOSLER H-J, Schwarz K, Ammann F and Gutscher H (2001) Computer simulation as a method of further developing a theory: Simulating the elaboration likelihood model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 201 — 215.
MOSLER H-J and Brucks W (2001) Social influence among agents. The simulation of social psychological theories. In N J Saam and B Schmidt (Eds.), Cooperative agents. Applications in the social sciences (125 — 147). Amsterdam: Kluwer.
PETTY R E and Cacioppo J T (1986) Communication and persuasion: central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer.
PETTY R E and Cacioppo J T (1986b) The Elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.) Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 123 — 205). New York: Academic Press.
PETTY R E and Cacioppo J T (1994) The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 69 — 81.
PETTY R E, Fleming M, and White P (1999) Stigmatized sources and persuasion: prejudice as a determinant of argument scrutiny. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(1), 19 — 34.
PETTY R E, Priester J R and Wegener D T (1994) Cognitive processes in attitude change. In R S J Wyer and T K Srull (Eds.), Handbook of Social Cognition (pp. 69 — 142). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
STAHLBERG D and Frey D (1993) Das Elaboration-Likelihood-Modell [The elaboration likelihood model]. In D Frey and M Irle (Eds.) Theorien der Sozialpsychologie. Band I: Kognitive Theorien (pp. 327 — 359). Bern: Hans Huber.
TANFORD S and Penrod S (1984) Social influence model: A formal integration of research on majority and minority influence process. Psychological Bulletin, 95 (2), 189 — 225.
TURNER J C (1991) Social influence. Bristol: Open University Press.
VAN DIJKUM C, de Tombe D and van Kuijk E (1999) Validation of simulation models. Amsterdam: SISWO.
WHICKER M L and Sigelman L (1991) Computer simulation applications. An introduction. London: Sage.
WOOD W, Lundgren S, Quellette J, Busceme S and Blackstone P (1994) Minority influence: a meta-analytic review of social influence processes. Psychological Bulletin, 115 (3), 323 — 345.
WOOD W, Rhodes N and Biek M (1995) Working knowledge and attitude strength: An information-processing analysis. In R E Petty and J A Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences (pp. 283 — 313). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Return to Contents of this issue
© Copyright Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, [2006]